lingvo.wikisort.org - Language

Search / Calendar

Denaʼina /dɪˈnnə/, also Tanaina, is the Athabaskan language of the region surrounding Cook Inlet. It is geographically unique in Alaska as the only Alaska Athabaskan language to include territory which borders salt water. Four dialects are usually distinguished:

  1. Upper Inlet, spoken in Eklutna, Knik, Susitna, Tyonek
  2. Outer Inlet, spoken in Kenai, Kustatan, Seldovia
  3. Iliamna, spoken in Pedro Bay, Old Iliamna, Lake Iliamna area
  4. Inland, spoken in Nondalton, Lime Village
Denaʼina
Denaʼina Qenaga, Denaʼinaqʼ
Native toUnited States
RegionAlaska (Cook Inlet region, Lake Clark, Lake Iliamna)
Ethnicity900 Denaʼina people (2007)[1]
Native speakers
5 (2020)[1]
Language family
Writing system
Latin (Denaʼina alphabet)
Official status
Official language in
 Alaska[2]
Language codes
ISO 639-3tfn
Glottologtana1289
ELPDena'ina
Tanaina is classified as Severely Endangered by the UNESCO Atlas of the World's Languages in Danger
This article contains IPA phonetic symbols. Without proper rendering support, you may see question marks, boxes, or other symbols instead of Unicode characters. For an introductory guide on IPA symbols, see Help:IPA.

Of the total Denaʼina population of about 900 people, only 75–95 members still speak Denaʼina. James Kari has done extensive work on the language since 1972, including his edition with Alan Boraas of the collected writings of Peter Kalifornsky in 1991. Joan M. Tenenbaum also conducted extensive field research on the language in the 1970s.


Ethnonym


The word Denaʼina is composed of the dena, meaning 'person' and the human plural suffix ina. While the apostrophe which joins the two parts of this word ordinarily indicates a glottal stop, most speakers pronounce this with a diphthong, so that the second syllable of the word rhymes with English 'nine' (as in the older spelling Tanaina).


Phonology


Denaʼina is one of seven Alaska Athabaskan languages which does not distinguish phonemic tone.


Consonants


The consonants of Denaʼina in practical orthography, with IPA equivalents.

Labial Dental Palatal Velar Uvular Glottal
plain lateral sibilant
Nasal m m n n
Plosive and
Affricate
plain (p b) t d t͡ɬ dl t͡s dz t͡ʃ j k g q gg ʔ '
aspirated t t͡ɬʰ tl t͡sʰ ts t͡ʃʰ ch k q
ejective t' t͡ɬʼ tl' t͡sʼ ts' t͡ʃʼ ch' k' q'
Fricative voiceless (f f) ɬ ɬ s s ʃ sh x x χ h h ĥ
voiced v v l l z z ʒ zh ɣ ŷ ʁ gh
Approximant (ɹ r)[lower-alpha 1] j y
  1. [ɹ] is only found in English loanwords.

Vowels


The 4 vowels of Denaʼina. Note that close vowels are more open in the environment of a uvular consonant.

Front Central Back
Close i u
Mid ə e
Open a

Generally, the vowels i, a, and u are considered 'long' vowels and are fully pronounced in words, however the e is considered a reduced vowel similar to the English schwa.


Syllable structure


In the Inland dialect, syllables at the end of a semantic unit are often longer, lower in pitch, and have longer rhymes. The onset of a syllable has consonant clusters of up to three, such as CCCVC, though these are rare and more commonly, a syllable onset is one or two consonants.


Morphology


Denaʼina is a polysynthetic language where a single word can mean the entirety of an English sentence.

ex:
nuntnghel'ił

nu-

again-

n-

you-

t-

FUT-

n-

see-

gh-

FUT-

sh-

I-

l-

CL-

'ił

see/FUT

nu- n- t- n- gh- sh- l- 'ił

again- you- FUT- see- FUT- I- CL- see/FUT

"I will see you again."

Verbs are the most elaborate part of speech in the Denaʼina language, which vary in verb paradigms which vary by subject, object, or aspect. The following example is of -lan the verb "to be" in the imperfective aspect and in the Nondalton dialect.

-lan, the verb "to be"
imperfective aspect
Nondalton dialect
Singular Plural
1st person

eshlan

eshlan

I am

ch'ilan

ch'ilan

we are

2nd person

inlan

inlan

you are

ehlan

ehlan

you all are

3rd person

nlan

nlan

he/she/it is

qilan

qilan

they are

areal

qilan

qilan

area is


Grammatical categories


Denaʼina indicates classification with obligatory verb prefixes, meaning the root verb appears at the end of the word. The verb will always specify a classification and often person, gender, or object prefixes that indicate aspects of the noun or object for transitive verbs, and aspects of the speaker for intransitive verbs. Person can also be indicated by suffixes on the noun; the singular person suffix on a noun is generally -en, whereas the plural suffix is generally -na or -ina. Plurals for non-persons that are animate are indicated by the noun suffixes -qa, -ha, and -yi. Inanimate plurals are unable to be indicated by a noun suffix, and instead attach to the verb.

For examples of person indication on the verb, see the chart under the morphology section above concerning the verb root -lan. Denaʼina specifies between 1st person singular/plural, 2nd person singular/plural, 3rd person singular/plural, and areal.

Verbs fall into many categories that are broadly lumped into "active" and "neuter", where an active verb indicates movement, a state of being incomplete, something being made, or in the production of sound, and a neuter verb indicates a general state of being that is complete. Categories of classification that are affixed to a verb also can refer to certain characteristics of the object of that verb. Depending on the gender affix that follows the classificatory affix, the nature of the object can change, as indicated by the following chart:

Classificatory verbsGender prefixes
∅-d-n-dn-q-
1Single compact object; ʼuball, trap, hat, sun, beaver lodgeegg, song, wordberry, bread, roe, coiled rope, headrock, ring, mirror, box, whetstonehouse, plot of land, situation, weather
2Elongated object; tunneedle, sled, boat, bow, gunpole, plate, cane, quill, pencil stickdentalium necklacemirrorx
3Enclosed object; łtunknife, full sack, rolled sleeping bagpillow, mattress, lakesack of berries, flour or fish eggsbox of rocksq+d ravine, valley
4Fabric-like object; kitsblanket, net, paper, open sleeping bag, empty sack, skin without hairskin with

hair, fur, caribou mat

5Object in open vessel; qusugar, water in containereggs or wood chips in bucketberries or roe in containerrocks or coal in bucketx
6Animate object; taperson, dog, doll, crucifixxxxx
7Food; kitpiece of meat, dry fishbeaver's food pileroexx
8Mushy object; tlaqʼmud, rotted food, wet cloth, butterwet tea leavesfish eggs (not in container)xpile of refuse, area of soft ground
9Plural objects; lutraps, boots, dogseggs, plates, cups, words, tobacco, songs, wavessg. uncoiled rope, pl. coiled ropes, beads, berries, roe, snarerocks, whetstones, boxeshouses, objects over area, freight
10Multiple objects; chuqʼsand, glacier icechips of woodberriesrocksearth, clouds

However, there are other categories of classification or instrumentation that indicate how an action was done or aspects about the outcome of the action. Many instrumental affixes have become causative over time. Causality is expressed by changing a classifier in the verb to "ł". Instrumental affixes that indicate the manner or motion of an action include the following: "-aqʼa", which refers to clubbing an object or leaving a depression in the snow; "-dni", which refers to causing an object to leave, disappear, or die; "-du", which refers to affecting an object with the mouth; "-eł,-eła, and -ł", which all indicate that the object being referred to was used in an instrumental sense; "-iqu (uqu)", which refers to a pointing motion; "-kʼ", which refers to a wiping motion; and "-lu", which refers to the use of a hand.


Space relations



Postpositions


Denaʼina shows space relations through the addition of morphemes that are either independent or bound, known as postpositions.

Postpositions can also be incorporated into a verb as a prefix.


Demonstratives


Space relations can also be marked by demonstratives pronouns, which indicate proximal/distal distinction.

For example:


Adverbs


Adverbs of location and direction can also convey space relations.


Directionals


The directional system in Denaʼina is based upon river flows, and are used with directional prefixes, roots, and suffixes.

For example, yunit means "at a place far upstream", and is composed of the prefix "yu", root "ni", and suffix "t".


Time and tense



Temporal adverbs


Temporal adverbs convey information about when an action or intent of the verb occurred.

For example:


Mode


Mode indicates when the action happens. Normal mode is also referred to as tense, and is given by mode/aspect prefix positions in the verbs. The tense modes are the imperfect (present), perfect (past) and the future represented by 4 types of imperfectives—∅, z, n, gh—and 4 types of perfectives—gh, z, n, ∅. There are also 4 modal variations: neuter, inceptive, optative-intentional, and negative.


Tense




Aspect


Aspect conveys information about how the action happened, and works in conjunction with tense. The most common aspects are conclusive, momentaneous, neuter, onomatopoetic, and semelfactive. The morpheme attached to the root verb can also change depending on aspect.

Aspect Mode
ImperfectPerfectFutureOptative
continuative yayayaYa
durative naxYałnaxnax
momentaneous yayał
neuter Ya'Ya'
repetitive nashnaknashnish

Modality


Modality is most commonly and easily seen in evidentials which usually appear at the end of a sentence.


Predicates, arguments, and case


Predicate refers to the main verb and auxiliary verbs, while arguments usually refer to those words outside of the predicate.

Word order in the basic Denaʼina sentence is subject-object-verb (SOV). Because of this, there is a low danger of referential ambiguity. It is rare to have both the subject and the objects as nouns; instead, one or both usually occur as pronouns. Some sentences differ from the SOV structure. In subject-verb (SV), the object is embedded in the verb as a pronoun, or the sentence doesn't require an object. In object-verb (OV), the subject is a pronoun contained in the verb, and the object is a noun.

An example sentence structure: object pronoun (argument) + outer subject pronoun (argument) + (other prefixes) + inner subject pronoun (argument) + ... verb stem (predicate). This example shows multiple arguments attaching to a single predicate.

A full clause can be expressed in the verb. In the verb, the verb stem is last, and even when embedded in the verb, the object and subject necessarily come first. In Denaʼina, all verbs require a nominative (subject) and an accusative (object), which indicates a nominative-accusative case. This means the marked morphemes, or those that change to convey more specific meanings, are those that indicate the object. Distinction occurs between the nominative and accusative, and each would have its own core argument.


Obliques


Obliques indicate instrumentals, locatives, and other arguments outside of core arguments. Both core and oblique arguments attach to the verb via prefixes which must occur in a certain order. In Denaʼina, obliques are prefixes to the verb which occur between the object prefix and the inner subject pronoun prefix and/or the outer subject pronoun.


Possession



Inherent possession


Denaʼina has inherently possessed nouns and non-verbs. Inherently possessed words consist of a prefix and a stem which are abound morphemes. For example, "shunkda" means "my mother", where "sh-" is the possessive pronoun meaning "my", and "-unkda" is the possessed root meaning "mother".

The pronouns used with inherently possessed bound morphemes:


Non-inherent possession


Non-inherent possession occurs with proper nouns as the possessor, shown by -a or -'a attached as a suffix to the possessed noun. Possessive pronouns are attached as a prefix to the possessed noun and the -'a is added at the end of the word.


Complement clauses


Complement clauses are clauses that act as the direct object of the verb, introduced by a complementizer (e.g.: in English, "that" or "which"). Complement clauses exist as subordinate clauses and bare clauses, as with other languages in the Athabaskan language family. Some complement clauses are marked by enclitics, and are always embedded as part of the sentence.

Relative suffixes are attached to nouns or verbs, and are one of the few suffixes on verbs. Common relative suffixes include:


Endangerment


The population of Denaʼina is 900. As of 2007, there are 75-90 speakers, and in 1970 there were only 10 speakers of the Kenai dialect.

Linguist Michael E. Krauss provides three levels of endangerment: safe; endangered, where the language is being learned by children but requires community effort to maintain it; and moribund, where the language is not being learned by children. According to this classification, the Denaʼina is a moribund language.


Revitalization efforts


There are various efforts to revitalize the language.

Contributing factors to the endangerment include the policy of early territorial schools to not let native students speak their own language, especially in regards to the Kenai dialect. This policy was often enforced via corporal punishment; the trauma caused elders, all within one generation, to avoid speaking the language.


References



Bibliography





На других языках


- [en] Denaʼina language

[ru] Танаина (язык)

Танаина (англ. Tanaina), также известный как денаина (самоназвание — Denaʼina [dənʌʔɪnʌ] — «много людей»[⇨]) или кенайский язык[5][6], — вымирающий атабаскский язык, на котором говорит одноимённый народ, проживающий у залива Кука и на прилегающих территориях на юге штата Аляска[6].



Текст в блоке "Читать" взят с сайта "Википедия" и доступен по лицензии Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike; в отдельных случаях могут действовать дополнительные условия.

Другой контент может иметь иную лицензию. Перед использованием материалов сайта WikiSort.org внимательно изучите правила лицензирования конкретных элементов наполнения сайта.

2019-2024
WikiSort.org - проект по пересортировке и дополнению контента Википедии