lingvo.wikisort.org - Language

Search / Calendar

Kalau Lagau Ya, Kalaw Lagaw Ya, Kala Lagaw Ya ([kala(u) laɡau ja]), or the Western Torres Strait language (also several other names, see below), is the language indigenous to the central and western Torres Strait Islands, Queensland, Australia. On some islands, it has now largely been replaced by Torres Strait Creole.

Kalau Lagau Ya
Western Torres Strait
Mabuiag
RegionWestern and Central Torres Strait Islands, Queensland
EthnicityBadu Island, Mabuiag, Kaurareg, Mualgal, Saibai Island, Boigu, Dauan Island, Kulkalgal, Maluigal
(Torres Strait Islanders)
Native speakers
957 (2016 census)[1]
Language family
Pama–Nyungan
  • Kalau Lagau Ya
Dialects
  • Kalau Lagau Ya, alt. Kalaw Lagaw Ya
  • Kalau Kawau Ya, alt. Kalaw Kawaw Ya
  • Kulkalgau Ya
  • Kaiwaligau Ya
Signed forms
Western Torres Strait Islander Sign Language
Language codes
ISO 639-3mwp
Glottologkala1377
AIATSIS[2]Y1
ELPKalaw Kawaw Ya
Linguasphere29-RG(A-a)
Range of Kalau Lagau Ya (orange) in the Torres Strait
This article contains IPA phonetic symbols. Without proper rendering support, you may see question marks, boxes, or other symbols instead of Unicode characters. For an introductory guide on IPA symbols, see Help:IPA.

Before colonisation in the 1870s–1880s, the language was the major lingua franca of the Torres Strait cultural area of Northern Cape York Australia, Torres Strait and along the coast of the Western Province/Papua New Guinea. It is still fairly widely spoken by neighbouring Papuans and by some Aboriginal Australians. How many non-first language speakers it has is unknown. It also has a 'light' (simplified/foreigner) form, as well as a pidginised form. The simplified form is fairly prevalent on Badu and neighbouring Moa.


Names


The language is known by several names besides Kalaw Lagaw Ya, most of which (including Kalaw Lagaw Ya) are names of dialects, spelling variants, dialect variants and the like — and include translations of the English terms, Western Island Language and Central Island Language:

Language name (with regional and spelling variants) English Notes
  • Kalaw Lagaw Ya / Kalau Lagau Ya / Kala Lagaw Ya
  • Kalaw Kawaw Ya / Kalau Kawau Ya
  • Kala Lagaw Langgus / Kala Lagau Langgus / Kalaw Lagaw Langgus / Kalau Lagau Langgus
Western Island Language
Lagaw Ya / Lagau Ya Home Island Language
  • Langgus
  • Linggo
Language, Lingo
Kaywalgaw Ya / Kaiwaligau Ya / Kawalgaw Ya Islanders' Language
Kowrareg (Kaurareg) Islander
Kulkalgau Ya Blood-Peoples' Language kulka 'blood' was an important Central Islands cult figure, and brother to Malo-Bumai of Mer.
Mabuiag Mabuiag Island
  • Westen
  • West Torres
  • Western Torres Strait
Western Torres Strait
  • Dhadhalagau Ya
  • Sentral / Central Islands
Mid-Island, Central Island Language

One term used by Eastern Islanders and neighbouring Papuans for Kala Lagaw Ya is Yagar Yagar, from the word yagar ( 'speech, etc.' + gár 'sympathy clitic' ('dear', 'please', etc.), often used by Western and Central Islanders in speech to show a sympathetic or nostalgic frame of mind.

In literature on the language the abbreviations KLY (Kalaw Lagaw Ya), KKY (Kalau Kawau Ya), KulY (Kulkalgau Ya), MY (Muwalgau Ya) and KY (Kaiwaligau Ya) are often used as abbreviations. The name Mabuiag /mabujaɡ/, in English pronounced /ˈmbiæɡ/, is fairly widespread as a name for the language, this having been established by the Cambridge Expedition to Torres Strait, whose main research on the language was with Mabuiag material. Though the preferred term in English in Academia for some time was Kala Lagaw Ya,[3] according to Ober (2007), the form was always regarded as "colloquial" by native speakers. In a High Court decision on 7 August 2013, the decision was taken to officially term the language Kalau Lagau Ya, using the formal form.

When speaking to each other, speakers generally refer to the language as Langgus 'language' or use phrases such as KLY/KulY ngalpudh muli, MY-KY ngalpudh/ngalpadh muli, KKY ngalpadh muliz "speak(s) our language", e.g. KLY/KulY ngalpudh muuli, thanamunungu tidailai!, MY-KY ngalpudh/ngalpadh muuli, thanamuningu tidailai!, KKY ngalpadh muli, thanamulngu tidaile! 'Speak in our language so they don't understand!'. Ngalpudh/ngalpadh literally means 'like us'. The construction X-dh mula+i- 'speak X-like' is used to refer to speaking in a language, e.g. KKY markaidh muliz 'speak [in] English', zapanisadh muliz 'speak [in] Japanese', dhaudhalgadh muliz 'speak [in] Papuan', mœyamadh muliz 'speak [in] Meriam Mìr', thanamudh muliz 'speak like them, speak [in] their language'. It is otherwise common for speakers to use nominal phrases like KLY/KulY ngalpun ya, MY-KY ngalpun/ngalpan ya, KKY ngalpan ya 'our language' to refer to the language when speaking to each other.


Geographic distribution


Map of the Torres Strait Islands.
Map of the Torres Strait Islands.

Kalau Lagau Ya is spoken on the western and central islands of Torres Strait, between Papua New Guinea (Naigay Dœgam Dhaudhai "North-side Mainland/Continent", also called Mœgi Dhaudhai "Small Mainland/Continent", KKY Mœgina Dhaudhai) and the Australian mainland (Zey Dœgam Dhaudhai "South-side Mainland/Continent", also known as Kœi Dhaudhai "Big Mainland/Continent"), though on some islands it has now been largely replaced by Brokan (Torres Strait Creole).

There is some folk history evidence that the language was spoken as a first language in a few villages neighbouring Torres Strait in Papua. It was also formerly spoken by the Hiámo (Hiámu, Hiáma) of Daru (Dhaaru) to the north-east of Torres Strait, who were originally settlers from Yama [Yam Island] in Torres Strait, Hiámu/Hiámo/Hiáma being a Kiwai pronunciation of Yama. The main body of the Hiámo moved to the Thursday Island group to escape the Kiwai colonisation of Daru some centuries ago.


Classification and external comparison


The language is classified as being part of the Pama–Nyungan languages. Mitchell (1995) and Mitchell (2015) regard it as a mixed language with an Australian core (PamaNyungan) and Papuan and Austronesian overlays, while Capell (1956) and Dixon (2002) classify it among the Papuan languages. The personal pronouns are typically Australian, most kin terms are Papuan, and significant sea/canoe and agricultural vocabulary is Austronesian.

Kalaw Lagaw Ya has only 6% cognation with its closest Australian neighbour, Urradhi, with a further 5% 'common' vocabulary (loans of various origins) — and about 40% common vocabulary with its Papuan neighbour, Meriam Mìr.[4] Of the 279 Proto-Paman forms given in Sommer (1969, pp. 62–66), only 18.9% have definite realisations in Kalaw Lagaw Ya, with a further 2.5% which may be present. One word that illustrates the problems of 'may-be' relationship is kùlbai (KKY kùlba) 'old', which may be a metathetic realisation of CA *bulgan 'big; old'. Potentially 80% of the vocabulary of the language is non-Australian, and includes Papuan and Austronesian items.[5] Bouckaert, Bowern and Atkinson (2018) found that Kalaw Lagaw Ya had the highest number of 'unique' (that is, non-Pama-Nyungan) forms of any Australian language in their sample.[6]

Australian
(Common Australian)
Papuan
(Proto–East Trans-Fly)
Austronesian
(Proto–Central District)
*nya-ga 'look'
nagai-/nage-/nagi- id.
*nyily 'name'
nel id.
*gamo 'belly'
gamu 'body'
*jana 'they'
thana id.
*p[ae]- 'that, there'
pi-/pe- 'specifically yonder'
*[w]aura 'trade wind, south-east trades'
wœur(a) id.
*ganyarra 'reptile'
kœnara 'k.o. tree snake'
*gabo 'cold'
gabu id.
*boro-ma 'pig'
bùrùm(a) id.
*galga 'spear'
kœlak(a) id.
*biro 'side'
bero 'rib; side of boat, hillside, river bank, etc.'
*pu[lr]i 'magic'
puy(i) (older puuři) 'magic, plant'

Oral tradition and cultural evidence recorded by Haddon (1935) and Laade (1968), backed by archaeological evidence[7] and linguistic evidence, shows that Austronesian trade and settlement in South-West Papua, Torres Strait and Cape York occurred; the languages have significant Austronesian vocabulary content,[8] including items such as the following:

Kalaw Lagaw YameaningMeriam MìrmeaningBine
(Papua)
meaningProto-Oceanic
Austronesian
meaning
maapu heavy(beberbeber)id.mæpuid.*mapaid.
maaludeep, deep water, sea, deep water beyond edge of reef~shallowsmaloid.maluid.*mwaloqsubmerged rock~reef (where it disappears into sea depths)
laba-cut, hack, strike (human)------------*la(m)pakstrike (as with sword or flat weapon), slam something down, slap
wœiwi mangowaiwi id. wiwiid.*waiwaiid.
waaku mat; sail papékid. waaku id.
(Kalaw Lagaw Ya loan)
*paquid.
waaru turtle(nam)id.waaru id.
(Kalaw Lagaw Ya loan)
*ponuid.

Some of the Austronesian content is clearly South-East Papuan Austronesian:

wordKalaw Lagaw YaGudang
(Australia)
Kiwai
(Papua)
Motu
(Central District, Papua)
Proto–SE PapuanProto-Oceanic
nacre, mother-of-pearlmaay(i)
(OKY maaři)
maarimarimairi*mwa[lr]i ?
outriggersayim(a)
OKY sařima
charimasarima
SE Kiwai harima
darima*nsarima*saRaman
pigbùrùm(a) boromaboroma*boro-ma*mporok
rope, cordwœru
KKY wœrukam(i)
uuruwarovaro*waro*waro
head, origin, base of tree, etc.kuik(u)
KLY kuiiku
--- PCD *quiqui id.*kulikuli*kulukulu 'head-end, upper part'

The linguistic history of the Torres Strait area is complex, and interaction of well over 2500 years has led to many layers of relationship between the local languages, including many words that are obviously common, such as the following 'trade' words in Torres Strait area languages.

Kalaw Lagaw YaMeriam MìrKiwai
(Papua)
Agöb
(Papua)
Gudang
(Australia)
Urradhi
(Australia)
Anguthimri
(Australia)
Mpakwithi
(Australia)
gii
tusk, knife, tusk/knife-life formation
gir
tusk/knife-life formation
giri
tusk, knife, tusk/
knife-life formation
 ? ?kiri/ghiri
knife
kiri
knife
kiri
knife
sœguba
tobacco
sogob
tobacco
suguba
tobacco
 ?[lower-alpha 1] tyughubha
tobacco
tyughubhu
tobacco
 ?
yœuth(a)
long house, hall; church
ìut (alt- eut)
church
 ? ?yutha
house
mœruka
any strange four-legged animal
 ? ?murruku
horse
 ?marruku
horse
mœrap(i)
bamboo
marepmarabo ?marrapimarrapi ?marrapi
eso
thanks
esoau ?eso ? ? ? ?
paaudh(a)
peace
paud ?piudapaaudha ? ? ?
warup(a)
drum
warupwarupa(w)arapawarrupa(w)arrupa(w)arrupa(w)arrupa
thuurik(a)
cutting tool
tulikturikaturika
Bine turi/turikæ
 ?thurriya
crowbar
thurriya
crowbar
thurriya
crowbar
  1. The only Gudang word recorded in the mid-1800s by Europeans was choki, from the Malay-based English Pidgin English used by the British (and other) sailors of the time. The Malay word is variously coki or cuki.

However, the question of external relationships of Kalaw Lagaw Ya is also complicated by resemblances between both the Paman (Pama-Nyungan, Australian languages) and the Trans-Fly (Papuan) languages. Though few, these may be significant, and include forms such as those noted below, not all of which appear in Kalaw Lagaw Ya. Such resemblances could point to a deep-level relationship dating back to before the flooding of Torres Strait at the end of the last age, as claimed by Mitchell[9] ,[4] or they could point to genetic inheritance and subsequent language contact, as discussed by Alpher, Bowern, and O'Grady 2009.[10]

Proto-Paman
(or a specific North Cape York language)
meaningProto-Trans-FlymeaningKalaw Lagaw Yameaning
*kaaluear*Vtkuruhearkaura;
kùrusai- (compounds only)
ear
*ŋaa(na)who*ŋanaid.ngaaid.
*minigood*mi:njiid.miinareal, true, very
anha
Urradhi, Gudang
breath*ŋanaid.ngœnaid.
wintamwintama
Urradhi
star*mpintomid.(thithuy(i)
OKY thithuri)
id.
*nyupunone*[ni/yi/dVr]ponVid.wœrapùn(i)
ùrapùn
(wara 'one of a group')
id.
*pamaman, person*pyamaid.(mabaig lit. 'walker')id.

Personal pronouns


A comparison of the Kalaw Lagaw Ya, Meriam Mìr, Kiwai and Uradhi personal pronouns show similarities and differences in typology. In comparison to Uradhi, Kalaw Lagaw Ya has an archaic typology — or, rather, Uradhi has innovated, having lost the Common Australian 1, 2 and 3 plurals. Kiwai does not have 1–2 pronouns, while Meriam Mìr does not have a dual and trial/paucal set of pronouns which correspond to its verb system. The Kalaw Lagaw Ya system, like that of Uradhi, is Australian:

Kalaw Lagaw Ya (KKY dialect)
number11–223
singularngai nginui masc
na fem
dualngalbengœbangipelpalai
pluralngœingalpangithathana
Meriam Mìr
number11–223
singularka mae
non-singularkimiwawi, i

Note that except for Meriam Mìr, the Trans Fly languages also have two-gender masculine-feminine systems, though not marked on the pronouns themselves.[11]

Kiwai
number123
singularmairainowai
dualnimotorigotoneito
trialnimoibirigoibineibi
pluralnimorigonei
Urradhi
number11–223
singularayu(va) antu(va)ulu(va)
dualampu(la)ali(va)ipu(la)ula(va)
plural(same as dual)ana(va)(same as dual)(same as dual)

However, even though the system has no real surprises for Australian linguistics, it is clear that Kalaw Lagaw Ya has innovated in the 1st and 2nd pronouns, which have the following CA origins:

The 2nd person dual and plural pronouns are based on forms that literally mean 'you dual' (ngipel) and 'you-they' (ngitha[na]), in much the same way as the demonstratives mark the dual and plural (see further below in Nominal Morphology).

The Kalaw Lagaw Ya pronouns and their Australian origins
EnglishKLYKulYKYKKYOld KY
(Kowrareg)
Proto-Pama–Nyungan origin
I ngay
stem: nga-
ngai
stem: nga-
ngai
stem: nga-
ngai
stem: nga-
ngai
stem: nga-
*ngayi
you and I ngœbangœbangœbangœbangœba*ngana+pulV
'we dual, exclusive'
we dual (exclusive)[lower-alpha 1] ngalbayngalbaingalbai/ngalbengalbengalbai/ngalbe*ngali+[?]
'you and me, you and us'
we (inclusive)[lower-alpha 1] ngalpa
stem: ngalpu-
ngalpa
stem: ngalpu-
ngalpa
stem: ngalpu-
ngalpa
stem: ngalpa-
ngalpa
stem: ngalpa-
*ngali+[?]
'we inclusive'
we (exclusive)[lower-alpha 1] ngœy
stem: ngœlmu-
ngœi
stem: ngœlmu-
ngœyi
stem: ngœymu-
ngœi
stem: ngœimu-
ngœři
stem: ngœři(mu)-
*ngali
'we inclusive'
you sing niningi/ningingi*NHiin
you dual nipel
stem: nipe-
nipel
stem: nipe-
ngipel
stem: ngipe-
ngipel/nipel
stem: ngipe-/nipe-
ngipel
stem: ngipe-
*NHiin+pulV 'you dual'
you pl nitha
stem: nithamu-
nitha
stem: nithamu-
ngitha
stem: ngithamu-
ngitha/nitha
stem: ngithamu-/nithamu-
ngithana
stem: ngithana(mu)-
*NHiin + *DHana 'they plural'
he nuy
stem: nu-
nui
stem: nu-
nui
stem: nu-
nui
stem: nu-
nui
stem: nu-
*NHu-
she nanananana*NHaan
they dual palay
stem: palamu-
palai
stem: palamu-
pale
stem: palamu-
palai
stem: palamu-,
Boigu pale
stem: palemu-
pale
stem: palamu-
*pula 'they dual, two'
they thana
stem: thanamu-
thana
stem: thanamu-
thana
stem: thanamu-
thana
stem: thanamu-
thana
stem: thanamu-
*DHana 'they plural'
who nganganganganga*ngaaNH
what
[lower-alpha 2]
mi-,
midha- (midhi-)
mi-mi-mi-mi-*miNHa 'food; what'
  1. Exclusive does not include the second person, i.e. 'you', while inclusive does.
  2. There is no independent nominative-accusative form for this pronoun.

Pre-historic overview


An examination of the various sub-systems (vocabulary, syntax, morphology) suggests the following:

Australian (Paman)

Some basic and abstract vocabulary, all personal pronouns (inc. who and what/which), some verbs. Some grammar, such as nominal and verb morphology (subject, agent, object, genitive, -l locative, -ka dative, perfective attainative, imperfective, -i/-iz(i) perfective active. These typological categories also exist in the Trans-Fly languages; the forms in Kalaw Lagaw Ya are clearly Australian.

Papuan (Trans-Fly)

Some basic and abstract vocabulary, some verbs. Some grammar, such as verb number and different stems for different number forms of some verbs. Use of state/movement verbs as existential and stative 'be' verbs. Two non-personal pronominals: naag/naga 'how', namuith 'when' (both in KKY, the dialect of the islands off the Papuan coast).

Austronesian

Some basic vocabulary, terminology dealing with agriculture, canoes, the weather, the sky and the sea, some abstract nouns, some verbs. Possibly some grammar in the form of function words, such as waadh (KKY waaza) 'existential emphasis' (i.e. 'it is true that ... '), Proto Oceanic Austronesian *waDa 'existential'.

The Australian word forms and structure found in Kalaw Lagaw Ya appear to be retentions, i.e. inherited; the original Australian forms appear to be unchanged at the core level. This suggests that the language is not a pidgin/creole in origin, but an Australian language which has undergone strong external lexical and grammatical influence. The language appears to be a classic case of shift,[12] whereby speakers of one language retained multilingualism over a long period of time, absorbing aspects of another language. The Austronesian and Papuan overlays modified the Australian phonology and syntax profoundly. The contrast of Australian laminal nh/ny and lh/ly and apical n and l has been lost, voicing has become phonemic and s, z, t, d, o and òò have developed. This also affected the phonology of Australian vocabulary, where these 'foreign' sounds also occur.

The non-Australian content appears to be mainly lexicon (including verbs), particularly dealing with the sea, farming, canoe and sky/weather/astrology, with possible some syntactic words. This presents a picture[12] of a typically extensive borrowing situation with much lexical borrowing and some structural borrowing with a large amount of passive bilingualism and little active bilingualism.

Laade's picture (1968) of Australian and Papuan settlement in Torres Strait supports the above scenario of Papuan and Austronesian speakers who shifted to an Australian language over a long period of time, the Austronesians being culturally a superstratum, however not in a position to impose their language. He presented folk history evidence that a few Austronesian traders (men) settled at Parema (north-east of Daru) and married local [Proto–Trans Fly speaking] women. To avoid further miscegenation, they soon moved and settled in Torres Strait, first to the Eastern Islands, then to the Central Islands, then to Moa, Badu and Mabuiag. At Mabuiag, Badu and Moa they found Aboriginal people, killed the men and kept the women (and presumably the children). Some moved on up to Saibai, Dœwan and Bœigu to avoid this new miscegenation, hence the lighter colour of many Saibai, Dœwan and Bœigu people. Bœigu folk history collected by Laade also shows direct East Austronesian genetic influence on Bœigu.[13]

The social context was that of a few Austronesian men who settled on the outskirts of an East Trans-Fly group, intermarried, and whose children were either bilingual, or speakers of their mothers' language, with some knowledge of their fathers' language. The local people did not need to speak the traders' language, who in turn had to speak the local language. The children in turn would then speak the local language, with varying ability in the fathers' language, particularly in areas that were culturally important for the fathers.

These people then shifted to Torres Strait — maintaining established ties with Papua as well as with Austronesian speakers further east (this latter being suggested by various characteristics of the Austronesian content in Kalau Lagaw Ya) — and overlaid an Australian population in such a way that the majority of women spoke an Australian language, with a significant number, mainly men, who spoke a South-East Papuan Austronesian language, accompanied by their Papuan wives and their perhaps bilingual children. Over time, the core structure of the local mothers' language dominated, with retention of the newcomers' Papuo-Austronesian content in the appropriate cultural subsystems. In essence this would have been a 'replay' of the original settlement by Austronesian traders at Parema, with the women understanding the language of the men, but not really needing to speak it while retaining parts of their language for significant areas. The children then created a new language shift to an Australian language with a Papuan-Austronesian admixture.

Kalaw Lagaw Ya is thus a mixed language in that a significant part of its lexicon, phonology and grammar is not Australian in origin. The core nominal, pronominal and verb morphology is Australian in both form and grammar — though a certain amount of the grammar is common to Trans-Fly and Paman languages in the first place. Some semantic categories, verb number morphology, and some other morphology are non-Australian in origin. Potentially 80% of its vocabulary is non-Australian. The interplay of the above within the subsystems of Kalaw Lagaw Ya lexicon, phonology and grammar points more to mixing through shift and borrowing rather than pidginisation and creolisation.


Outside influences


The language also has some vocabulary from languages outside the Torres Strait area, from the Indonesian, Malay, Philippine, English and other 'outsiders'. Where loan words from the Western Austronesian (Indonesian, etc.) loans are concerned, it is possible that some such came into the language in pre-European contact days, with the Makassans and similar fishermen/traders who visited northern Australia and Torres Strait.

Examples of post-European contact Western Austronesian loan words:

wordKalaw Lagaw Yaorigin
coconut toddythúbatuba (Eastern Indonesian or Philippine language)
trumps (in cards)záruzaru/jaru (Eastern Indonesian or Philippine language)
mate, friend, brotherbala
Boigu variants: bœra, baya
bela/bala (Eastern Indonesian or Philippine language)
blachanbœlasanMalay: belacan

Some words in the language, assuming that they are Western Austronesian loans, appear to be pre-contact words. This is suggested by form and use in the language and in neighbouring languages (some of these words may ultimately be from Arabic and Sanskrit).[14]

Kalaw Lagaw Yameaningpossible sourcemeaning
aya (KKY)
aye (KLY,KulY,KY)
come! (singular)Malay: ayocome!
thurik(a)cutting toolTetun: tudikknife
ádhi
  • huge, great (also as an honorific)
  • story (with cultural, religious or similar significance)
  • 'story stone or rock', i.e. a rock or stone that represents someone or something with sacred or cultural aignificance
Malay: adi
(Sanskrit: अधि, romanized: adhi)
huge, great
(also as an honorific)
kœdal(a)crocodileMalay: kadal
Makassarese: kaɖalaq
lizard
pawadeed, action, customMalay: paal [paʔal]
(Arabic: فَعَلَ, romanized: faʿala)
deed, action

Loans from modern Eastern Austronesian (Polynesian and Melanesian) into the language are mainly of religious or 'academic' use. In general, such words are terms for objects that are strictly speaking European goods. One exception is the last in the following table, which is commonly used instead of the traditional words imi 'spouse's opposite-sex sibling', 'opposite sex sibling's spouse' and ngaubath 'spouse's same-sex sibling', 'same-sex sibling's spouse'. These have also similarly been replaced in common usage by the English loan woman (pronounced [woman]) in the meaning of 'sister/daughter-in-law'.

Kalaw Lagaw Yameaningsourcemeaning in originating language
thúsibook, document, letter, etc.Samoan: tusi(same meaning)
laulautableSamoan: laulauplaited coconut leaf used as a tray
wakasuanointment oilDrehu: wakacucoconut oil
thawiyan
(emotive form thawi)
brother/son-in-lawVanuatu: taweanbrother-in-law

Other biblical loans are from Ancient Greek, Latin and Biblical Hebrew:

Kalaw Lagaw Yameaningsourcemeaning in originating language
basalayakingdomAncient Greek: βασιλείᾱid.
arethoholy communionAncient Greek: ἄρτοςwheaten bread
SathanaSatanBiblical Hebrew: שטןSatan, opponent, adversary
Sabadh(a), SabadhiSundayBiblical Hebrew: שבתSaturday (Sabbath)

Two early English loans of interest show back formation from what in the language appeared to be a plural. Most nouns (a) form the plural with an -l suffix, and (b) in the nominative-accusative singular elide the stem final vowel, thus tukuyapa- 'same-sex sibling', plural tukuyapal, nominative-accusative tukuyap. Under this model 'custard-apple' became katitap, plural katitapal, and 'mammy-apple' (pawpaw/papaya) became mamiyap, plural mamiyapal.


Dialects


There are four main dialects, two of which are on probably the verge of extinction, one (Kaiwaligau Ya) through convergence to the neighbouring Kalaw Lagaw Ya. Within the dialects there are two or more subdialects. The average mutual intelligibility rate, based on a Swadesh count, is around 97%.

The Southern dialect has certain characteristics that link it closely to the northern dialect, and folk history dealing with the Muralag group and Mua reflects this, in that the ancestors of the Kowrareg (the Hiámo) originally came from Dharu (Daru, to the north east of Torres Strait) — and who had previously settled on Dharu from Yama in Central Torres Strait.[16]


Samples of the dialects


    They cut down a big tree earlier today to make a canoe.

    Kalau Kawau Ya: Thana kayb kœi puy pathanu gulpa aymœipa.
    Kalaw Lagaw Ya: Thana kayib kœi puuyi pathanu gulka ayimka.
    Kulkalgau Ya: Thana kayb kœi puy pathanu[l] gulka aymœika.
    Kaiwaligau Ya/Muwalgau Ya: Thana kayib kœi puy pathanu[l] gulpa aymaipa.
    Old Kaiwaligau Ya (Kowrareg): Thana kayiba kœi puuři pathanulai gulpa[ri] ayimařipa[ri].
    Simplified Kalaw Lagaw Ya: Thana kaib kœi puy pathai gulka aymaik.

    Underlying form:

    Thana+ kayiba∅ kœi puuRi+ patha+++nulai gul+ka/pari ayima+[R]i+ka/pari
    They pl+nom today big tree+acc chop+att+sing+today pst] canoe+dat make+vn+dat

    Some isolect markers of the four dialects of Kalaw Lagaw Ya:

    Kalau Kawau YaKaiwaligau YaKalaw Lagaw YaKulkalgau YaKauraraigau Ya
    (Kowrareg)
    you sing ngingininingi
    house laaglaag,
    mùdh
    mùùdhamùdhlaaga,
    mùdha
    thunder gigidhuyumdhuyumdhuyumdhuyuma
    end, finish muasi-
    (B muyasi-)
    muasi-minasi-minasi-moasi-
    heat komkœmànkœmàànakomkœmàna
    steam kœmankœmànkœmàànakœmànkœmàna
    Dative -pa-pa
    (-ka)
    -ka
    (-pa)
    -ka
    (-pa)
    -pa, -pari
    (-ka)
    Ablative -ngu(z),
    -z(i)
    -ngu,
    -z(i)
    -ngu,
    -zi
    -ngu,
    -z(i)
    -nguzi,
    -zi
    Present Perfective
    Active Singular
    -iz,
    -izi, -izin
    -i (Badhu -in),
    -izi (Badhu -izin)
    -i,
    -izi
    -i,
    -izi
    -izi,
    -iziři

    Dialectal differences



    Phonology

    Phonological differences between the dialects are rare, and in general sporadic. The only regular differences are the following:


    Colloquial final unstressed vowel elision

    Found in Kulkalgau Ya and Kaiwalgau Ya:

    Such elision is rare or sporadic in Kalau Kawau Ya.


    Final unstressed vowel devoicing

    In Kalaw Lagaw Ya, such final vowels in correct language are devoiced, and deleted in colloquial language, except in a small class of words which include bera 'rib', where there is a short vowel in the stem and in which the final vowel is permanently deleted, with compensatory lengthening of the final consonant (thus berr).

    Strictly speaking, the process is not final vowel devoicing, but rather stressed vowel lengthening accompanied by final vowel devoicing — except in the case of words such as bera 'rib' > berr, where the process is final consonant lengthening by the final vowel being 'incorporated' into the consonant. Note that in the following the word-final capital letter represents a devoiced vowel:

    In declined forms of such words, the long vowel is shortened, and the final vowel voiced, and in words like ber 'rib' the final vowel often reappears:

    This vowel shortening in affixed/modified forms exists in all dialects, however the other dialects have retained contrastive length to some extent, whereas Kalaw Lagaw Ya has largely lost it for 'morphophonological' length, where the stressed vowel in non-emotive words (see below) of one or two syllables is automatically lengthened in the nominative-accusative; this also applies to words of three syllables with second syllable stress (as in mœrààpI 'bamboo').

    One of the very few length contrasts in the Kalaw Lagaw Ya dialect is kaaba 'dance performance, knot in bamboo etc.' vs kaba, kab 'paddle, oar' (Old Kaiwaligaw Ya [Kauraraigau Ya] kœRaba; œRa has regularly given short a in Kalaw Lagaw Ya in kaba, kab). Such length contrasts are more widespread in the other dialects.

    The exceptions are (1) the small class or words that include ber 'rib' and kab 'oar, paddle', and (2) emotive words. Emotive words are those that equate to a certain extent to diminutives in languages such as Irish, Dutch and German, where specific suffixes are added to show 'diminutive' status (-ín, -je and -chen/-el/-lein respectively). Emotive words include familiar kinship terms [the equivalent of English Mum, Dad and the like] and words used in emotive contexts such as singing/poetry.

    WordNon-EmotiveEmotive
    Mum (apuuwa, apùù, àpu — mother)Ama
    Dad (thaathi, thaath — father)Baba
    child kaazi, kaazkazi
    wife iipi, iipipi
    home (island) laaga, laaglaga
    dust, spray pœœya, pœœypœya, paya
    bamboo mœrààpi, mœrààpmœràpi, marapi
    head kuwììku, kuwììkkuwìku, kuiku

    Final i-glide deletion

    A small class of words in Kalau Kawau Ya do not have the final i-glide found in the other dialects, including the following:

    Word forms in neighbouring languages as well in the Kauraraigau Ya (Kowrareg) of the mid-to-late 19th century, such as the Meriam Mìr kopor and Kauraraigau Ya kupar/kopar 'birth cord' show that in such words the final -i/Ø are the modern forms of older .


    Syntax


    The main syntactic differences are:


    Verb negative construction

    In all dialects except Kalau Kawau Ya, the verb negative is the nominalised privative form of the verbal noun. As this form in itself a noun, its subject and direct object are cast in the genitive:

    The Kalau Kawau Ya dialect uses the verbal noun privative form as an invariable verb negative:


    Verb tenses/aspects

    The Kalau Kawau Ya dialect has the tenses and aspects listed in the section on verb morphology. The other dialects have largely lost the remote future tense, using the habitual instead; the remote future in the other dialects is retained most commonly as a 'future imperative', where the imperative refers to a vague period in the future. The Kalaw Lagaw Ya dialect also has a 'last night' tense, where the adverb bungil/bungel (reduced form bel) 'last night' has become a verb postclitic, following the model of the adverb ngùl 'yesterday', which had previously become grammaticalised as a 'recent past' tense marker in all dialects, with reduction to -ngu in Kalau Kawau Ya. In the other dialects bongel 'last night' is a fully functioning temporal adverb used in conjunction with either the today past or the recent past.

    The dialects differ in the forms of the following affixes:

    1. present imperfective/near future perfective/verbal noun dative:
      KKY/KY -pa, KLY/KulY -ka
    2. Recent past
      KKY -ngu, KLY/KY/KulY -ngul
    3. Today past
      KKY/KLY/KulY -nu, KY -nul (older -nulai)
    4. Habitual
      KKY -paruig/paruidh/-parui/-paru/-pu (-pu most commonly on stems of two or more syllables, and the bi-syllabic forms on stems of one syllable [the consonant final forms are emphatic forms])
      KLY/KulY -kuruig
      KY -kurui

    Nominal affixes

    The main nominal affix difference is the dative ending, which has the following forms in the various dialects:

    The plural/HAVE suffix -LAI (underlying form) also shows a small amount of dialect variation with stems of two syllables, where Kulkalgau Ya differs from the other dialects in retaining the full form of the suffix -lai, reduced to -l in the other dialects. In stems of three or more syllables, the suffix is reduced to -l in all dialects, while retained as -lai (variants according to noun sub-class -thai, -ai, -dai) with stems of one syllable.

    Three+ syllable stem

    burum 'pig', stem: buruma-, plural burumal

    Bisyllabic stem

    lag, KLY laaga 'place, home, home island', stem: laga-, plural lagal, KulY lagalai

    Monosyllabic stems
    1. Regular vowel final: ma 'spider', plural malai
    2. Regular -i glide final: mui 'fire', plural muithai, KLY muithail
    3. Regular -l final: pel 'fish tail', plural pelai
    4. Regular -r final: wœr/wur/uur 'water', plural wœlai/wulai/ulai, KKY wœrai
    5. Irregular vowel final stem: ya 'speech, word(s), message, language, etc.', plural yadai, KLY yadail

    Vocabulary

    The main differences between the dialects are to do with vocabulary, as can be seen in the following examples:


    Phonology



    Consonants


    Kala Lagaw Ya is the only Australian language to have the alveolar fricatives /s/ and /z/. However, these have allophonic variants /tʃ/ and /dʒ/, which are the norm in Australian languages (usually /c/ and /ɟ/ but non-contrasting). These latter two are allophones in that in all environments /s/ and /z/ can appear, while /tʃ/ and /dʒ/ can not appear at the end of a word; note that this allophony is very similar to that of the neighbouring Papuan language Bine. All the stops, except for the alveolars t and d, have fricative allophones, thus p can be [p] or [ɸ], k can be [k] or [x], b [b] or [β], and so on. Furthermore, it is one of the few Australian languages with fully functioning voiced-voiceless distinctions (p/b, t/d, s/z, k/g, th/dh) — and one of the few without retroflex stops.

    The language is also one of the few Australian languages with only one rhotic, one l and one n. The earliest recorded dialect, Kaiwalgau Ya (Kauraraigau Ya [Kowrareg]), however, did have two rhotics, the tap and the glide; the rhotic glide has in general become /j/, /w/ or zero in the other dialects (and Modern Kaiwaligau Ya), rarely /r/. Neighbouring languages retain an /r/ in related words, such as:

    However, in singing, /s/, /z/ and /r/ are pronounced [s], [z], and [ɹ], are virtually never as [tʃ], [dʒ] and [r].

    Obstruent Nasal Approximant
    voiceless voiced
    Labial p pb bm mw w
    Dental th dh n l
    Alveolar t td d r r
    Alveo-palatal s/ sz/ z j y
    Velar k kɡ gŋ ng

    Note:

    1. The consonant /d/ varies to some extent with /r/, particularly in KKY/KY kadai-/karai-, KLY/KulY kad[a]/kad[a]/kadai/karai 'upwards'.

    Vowels


    Unrounded Rounded
    short long short long
    Close i i iiu u uu
    Close-mid e e eeʊ ù ʊː ùù
    Open-mid ə œ əː œœo o oo
    Open a a aaɔ ò ɔː òò

    Notes:

    1. The long vowel ùù is only found in Kala Lagaw Ya.
    2. Length is to a certain extent contrastive, and partly allophonic.
    3. The +/-round contrast is reminiscent of Papuan phonology.
    4. The mid long vowels are allophonic variants of the mid short vowels that are in the process of developing phonemic status, while the short vowel ò is similarly in origin an allophone of òò.

    Internal reconstruction and comparison with neighbouring languages suggests an underlying four vowel structure with contrasting vowel length, where underlying *i typically gives surface i and e, underlying *a typically gives surface a and œ, underlying typically gives surface o and ù, and underlying *u typically gives surface ù and u (there are other realisations as well, depending on rules of assimilation etc.):

    Underlying Vowels-round+round
    +high *i,*ii*u,*uu
    -high *a,*aa*o,*oo

    The language undergoes low-level vowel shifts, caused by stress domination within words and phrases. Long vowels are shortened, and short vowels raise when the word is preceded by morphemes such as adjectives, demonstrative articles, prefixes and the like; the changes also occur within words when these are suffixed:

    The processes are low-level in that they are not 'automatic' — the changes do not have to occur and can be consciously 'blocked'. In normal speech, vowel shortening and the change of a to œ normally occur, while the changes of e to i and o to u are sporadic, and most common in unstressed syllables.

    Assimilation of vowels to other vowels in the vicinity and consonants is also widespread, particularly of the vowel œ:


    Kauřařaigau Ya phonology


    The following summary of the phonology of Old Kauraregau Ya is compiled from MacGillivray (1852), Brierly (in Moore 1978), Ray and Haddon (1897) and Ray (1907). In general, there does not to appear to have been any great phonological difference between OKY and the modern dialects of Kalau Lagau Ya (apart from the retention of ř).


    Stress

    Stress appears to have been similar to that of the modern dialects, with stress patterns being most similar to that of modern Bœigu and Ngœrupai speech, the most conservative dialects in this respect. In the following the standardised forms are in bold.


    Bisyllabic forms

    Stress is initial:

    A few forms (such as gru: gœrú 'sugar cane') show that contrastive stress existed in bisyllabic words.


    Multisyllabic forms

    Stress is either on the initial or second syllable:

    (1) initial:

    (2) second:

    Shifted stress also appears to have occurred as in the modern dialects:


    Vowels and diphthongs

    These appear to have been the same as in the modern language. Vowel length in general appeared in the same environments as in KKY, though some amount of vowel lengthening under the KLY model is evident, as in kawp: kaapu 'seed', Kalau Kawau Ya / Kulkalgau Ya kapu, Kalau Lagau Ya kaapu.

    The exact extent of retention of underlying vowel length and the development of variant forms is difficult to measure, as the spelling systems used by Brierly and MacGillivray did not always mark vowel length. Further, as they obtained words through elicitation (which has a common 'lengthening effect' on vowels when words are 'slowed down'), there are a few cases where they marked vowel length wrongly. Ray marked vowel shortness in stressed syllables.

    The various sound changes that the vowels and diphthongs undergo in the modern language also occurred in OKY. One change that occurred much more than in the modern dialects was that of ai monophthongisation to e. The resulting e then often raised to i in open unstressed syllables.

    No change:

    Change:

    In the modern dialects, these forms are:

    The change of ai to ei appears to have been very common elsewhere in the dialect:

    One form shows optional i insertion:

    gassumu-, gassima-: gasama- ~ gasœma- ~ gasima- 'catch, get', modern dialects gasama- ~ gasœma-


    Development of ř


    OKY had one more consonant than modern WCL, transcribed ř. Though the actual pronunciation of this sound and its difference from r was not given by any early writer, it most likely was a rhotic glide [ɹ], perhaps with a palatal 'hue'. The loss of this sound in the other dialects (and in modern KY) occurred in the following rules; the changes were beginning to be evident already in OKY:

    Ř between like vowels or in [ə]__V deletes.

    Ř sporadically becomes [+hi] when in ə__a and the following syllable is stressed.

    Ř becomes a [+V] glide when between [-hi] and [+hi] vowels, and between [+bak] and [-bak] vowels.

    Vuř becomes /w/ when intervocalic.

    Ř optionally becomes /i/ when syllable final and following [-hi] vowels; in at least two words metathesis first occurred.

    Ř deletes when syllable final following high vowels and non-final.

    Ř disappears when followed by unstressed i and more than one syllable.

    Early spellings (e.g. möaga [məaga] 'sweat' and neet/naat/nöat/niet [nejat], [nat], [nəat], [nijet] 'platform' show that ř disappeared first, leaving a hiatus (except in those cases where ř > y~i), with reduction of [V1-V1] and [ə-V1] to [V1], and [ə-VV] to [VV].

    OKY underwent the same allophony and sound changes as the modern dialects, though z ~ dh and s ~ th variation appears to have been more general in OKY, as in the following (perhaps evidence of older allophony in the language which is now levelling out):

    An instance of optional r deletion before s is also attested in the following example, unless the first i in myaichipp is a misprint or misreading of *myarchipp:

    maayi-arsipa 'wail, keen, weep': Brierly myaichipp, MacGillivray maierchipa, OKY mayarsipa, mayasipa

    Various forms in OKY showed metathesis of ř and r in the environment of u, i and au:


    Syllabification


    Syllabification occurred as in the modern dialects, with the addition of ř also attested as a syllable final consonant. One word was recorded by Brierly and MacGillivray with a [+nas][-son] cluster, namely enti 'spider', however this appears to be a confusion; enti is probably Gudang (Australia) ant[h]i 'sore'.

    Syllables were vowel final or end in r, ř, l, glide i or glide u. Otherwise surface syllable final consonants have an underlying following vowel, in which case all consonants could be syllable initial.


    Orthography


    There is no strict standard spelling, and three slightly different orthographies (and often mixes of them) are in use.


    Mission Spelling


    The Mission Spelling (established at first by Loyalty Islands missionaries in the 1870s, then modified by Polynesian missionaries in the 1880s): a, b, d, e, g, i, j, k, l, m, n, ng, o, ö, p, r, s, t, u, z, sometimes also th, dh, dth, tr, dr, oe, ë, w, y, j, and sometimes double vowels to show length. This spelling system was based on that used for the Drehu (Lifu) language, though later with the change to Polynesian mission staff, as well as the growing number of indigenous Torres Strait missionaries, the overtly Drehu forms tr, dr and ë were lost; these had no phonological basis in Kalaw Lagaw Ya. The mission system is used in the Reports of the Cambridge Expedition to the Torres Strait (Haddon et al., 1898 and on, University of Cambridge) and in Myths and Legends of Torres Strait (Lawrie, University of Queensland, 1971). Ray, the linguist of the Cambridge Expedition, also used various diacritics to represent short vowels and vowel quality.


    Klokheid and Bani


    Established in the 1970s: a, aa, b, d (alveolar), dh (dental), e, ee, g, i, ii, k, l, m, n, ng, o, oo,oe (/ə/), ooe (/əː/), p, r, s, t (alveolar), th (dental), u, uu, w, y, z


    Saibai, Boigu, Dauan students


    Established in the late 1970s: a, b, d (alveolar), dh (dental), e, g, i, k, l, m, n, ng, o, oe (/ə/), p, r, s, t (alveolar), th (dental), u, w, y, z (vowel length, though it exists, is rarely represented).

    People not only use these three slightly differing spelling systems, but also write words more or less as they pronounce them. Words are therefore often spelt in various ways, for example sena/sina 'that, there', kothai/kothay/kothei/kothey/kothe 'back of head, occiput'. Such variation depends on age, family, island, village and other factors such as poetic speech. It can be difficult at times to decide which is most correct — different people have different opinions (and sometimes have very strong opinions).

    In general the pronunciation of older people has priority; however, some people can actually get quite offended if they think the language is written the 'wrong' way. Some insist that the mission spelling should be used, others the Bani spelling, and still others the KKY (Saibai etc.) spelling, and still again others use mixes of two or three, or adaptations thereof. Some writers of the Mabuiag-Badhu dialect (Kalaw Lagaw Ya), for example, write mainly in the Mission system, sometimes use the digraphs oe, th, dh (variant dth) and sometimes use capital letters at the ends of words to show devoiced vowels, such as ngukI 'fresh water/drinking water, fruit juice' /ŋʊːki̥/. In the Bani/Klokheid orthograophy nguki is written nguuki, and in the other dialects the final vowel is either fully voiced, nguki /ŋʊki/), or elided, nguk /ŋʊk/).

    The biggest bone of contention between the advocates of the 'modern' orthographies and the 'traditionalist' orthographies is the use of w and y to show the semi-vowels. In general native speakers in literacy classes seem to find y and w very difficult to learn, and that u and i are the 'logical' letters to use. Syllabification of words by untrained speakers suggests that u and i are really the underlying sounds. Thus, a word like dhaudhai/dhawdhay 'mainland, continent' syllabifies as dha-u-dha-i, not dhau-dhai. In songs, the glide-u/i can also be given full syllable status. Historical considerations also point to the semi-vowels often being vocalic rather than consonantal. Thus, lagau, the genitive of laag[a] 'place' is in underlying form <laaga+ngu>; the full form of the genitive ending -ngu is only retained where the nominal has a monosyllabic stem (see the section on Nominal Morphology). Similarly, verbal nouns end in -i, e.g. lumai, stem luuma- 'search, look for, seek, hunt'. The mid-19th century to early 20th century records of Kauaraigau Ya show that the verbal noun ending was previously -ri (thus lumari), where the -r- was presumably the rhotic glide rather than the rhotic tap/trill.

    A dictionary now in preparation (Mitchell/Ober) uses an orthography based on detailed study of the surface and underlying phonology of the language, as well as on observation of how people write in real life situations. It is a mix of the Mission and Kalau Kawau Ya orthographies with the addition of diacritics (the letters in brackets) to aid correct pronunciation, since many of the people who will use this dictionary will not be speakers of the language:

    a (á), b, d, dh, e (é), g, i (í), k, l, m, n, ng, o (ó, ò, òò), œ (œ'), r, s, t, th, u (ú, ù), w, y, z

    Within this orthography, w and y are treated as consonants — this is their phonological status in the language — while u and i are used as the glides where phonological considerations show that the 'diphthong' combination has vocalic status.

    The typewritten forms of œ and œœ are oe and ooe.


    Pronunciation of the letters


    The English pronunciations given in the list below are those of Australian English, and are only meant as a guide. The letters in square brackets ([]) are the IPA.

    Combinations of vowels ('diphthongs', such as ai, au, œi, eu etc.) are pronounced as written. Thus, for example, ai is a-i (basically very similar to 'i' in 'mine' with a posh accent). In singing and sometimes in slow speech, such vowel combinations can be said separately. In the Bani and Saibai (etc.) orthographies, the last elements can be written as y and w instead of i and u. The diphthongs are:


    Grammar



    Nominal morphology


    Where the morphology is concerned, the language is somewhere along the continuum between agglutinative and fusional. Nominals have the following cases: nominative, accusative, instrumental (subsumes ergative), dative (subsumes allative, purposive), ablative (subsumes elative, avoidative), specific locative, nonspecific locative (subsumes perlative and comitative) and global locative. Nominals also have the following derived forms: privative, similative, resultative and proprietive, which also forms the noun nominative-accusative plural. All stems end in a vowel or a semi-vowel, except for a few monosyllables ending in -r and -l (which includes the very few reduplicated words, like tharthar 'boiling, seething', as well as ngipel 'you dual' [a compound of ngi 'you singular' and -pal 'two']). For many nouns the surface nominative(-accusative) undergoes a final stem-vowel deletion rule; in the Kalaw Lagaw Ya dialect the rule results in final devoiced vowels accompanied by main vowel lengthening. There are three numbers, singular, dual and plural. Singular and dual are the same form in all nominals except the personal pronouns. Furthermore, the plural is only distinguished in the nominative-accusative — except for the personal pronouns, where the difference in number is shown by the stem.

    There are two nominal classes, Common Nominals (common nouns, demonstratives, locative/temporal/etc. adverbs) and Proper Nomals (Proper names [personal names, boat names, emotive kinship terms], pronouns). The major difference between the two classes are 1) semantic — Proper nominals have pronominal characteristics, and, 2) declensional, for example Proper Nominals have one locative case rather than the three of Common Nominals.


    Common Nominal declensions

    Note that the following are in the Kalau Kawau Ya dialect.

    Case/SuffixHoe/AdzePlace/HomeKnifeWaterMudMiddlelookinggiving, getting, being, moving, doing, etc.
    stem typemultisyllabic
    -u final
    multisyllabicmonosyllabic
    vowel final
    monosyllabic
    -r/-l final
    monosyllabic
    -i glide final
    locative nominal
    (adverb)
    multisyllabic
    verbal noun
    monosyllabic
    verbal noun
    stempábu-lága-gi-wœr-sái-dhadha-naga+i-má+i-
    nom-acc sing-dual pábulággiwœrsáidhadh[lower-alpha 1]nœgaimái
    nom-acc pl pabullagalgilaiwœraisaithaidhadhal[lower-alpha 1]
    inst pabunlaganginu/gínwœrnu/wœransaithudhadhannœgainmain
    gen pabulagauginguwœrngusaingudhadhaunœgaimai
    (maingu)
    dat pabupalagapagipawœrpasaipadhadhapanœgaipamaipa
    abl pabungulaganguginguwœrngusaingudhadhaznœgailemaithaile
    sp-loc pabunu, pabu'laganu, laga'gilai, ginuwœrai, wœrnusaithai, saithedhadhal, dhadha'nœgainumainu
    n-sp-loc pabuyalagayagiyawœriyasaiyadhadhayanœgaiyamaiya
    gl-loc pabuyablagayabgipuwœrab, wœrpusaiyab, saipudhadhayabnœgaiyamaiya
    prop pabul(ai)lagal(ai)gilaiwœraisaithai,
    saithe
    dhadhal(ai)[lower-alpha 1]nœgail(ai)maithai
    priv pabugilagagigigiwœrgisaigidhadhagi[lower-alpha 1]nœgaigimaigi
    sim pabudh(a)lagadh(a)gidhawœrdha/wœradhsaidh(a)dhadhadh(a)
    [lower-alpha 1]
    nœgaidh(a)maidh(a)
    res pabuzilagazigiziwœrzisaizidhadhazi[lower-alpha 1]nœgaizimaizi
    1. In compounds only.

    Irregular nouns


    There are few irregular nouns, the most common being:

    1. ai 'food', ya 'speech, language, message, etc.', li 'basket', lu 'mound, bump, hump' (instrumental aidu, yadu, lidu, ludu; specific locative/proprietive-plural aidai/aide, yadai, lidai, ludai)
    2. KKY na, KLY naawu, KulY/KY nawu 'song'; KKY yu 'drying rack, cooking rack' (other dialects nuuwa, nu); specific locative/proprietive-plural KKY nathai, KLY/KY nawul, KulY nawlai; KKY yuthai (other dialects nuwanu, nuwa; nuwal, KulY nuwalai))
    3. za 'thing, object, matter, etc.' This word has a fuller stem form, zapu-, which appears in certain forms: instrumental zapun; genitive zapu; proprietive-plural zapul. In the locative forms both stems (za- and zapu-) appear: specific locative zanu, zapunu, etc.
    4. gœiga 'sun, day'; bireg/bereg 'shelf'. The stems of these words have different forms to the nominative-accusative: gœiga — stem: gœigœyi-, gœigi-; bireg/bereg — stem: bœreigi-, biregi-
    5. dœgam, KLK dœgaamu 'side, direction, point of compass, aspect'. This word has two stem forms, in free variation: dœgamu-, daguma-

    Demonstratives


    The language has a closed class of demonstrative morphemes with special morphological characteristics:

    Prefixes
    Stems

    The Kauřařaigau Ya forms recorded are the same as in the modern dialects, with the exception of ka-/kařu- 'non-specific here, this', se-/si-/seřu- 'there, that', kařa- 'non-specific yonder', modern dialects kai-, %ka- and -puwai 'ahead there', modern dialects -pai/-pa.

    These demonstratives can take masculine, feminine and non-singular morphology (as such are pronominal) as well as case forms. Í- 'here, this' and se/si- 'there, that (not too far away)' take the gender/number morphemes as suffixes, and the other demonstratives take them as prefixes. Note that ka- 'non-specifically here' and kai- 'there in the distance in a non-specific position' cannot appear with the gender/number morphemes, these latter being specific. Í- and se/si- also take an article forming affix -bi to become demonstrative articles (e.g. KLY senuubi kaazi, KKY senaubi kaz 'that boy', KLY senaabi kaazi, KKY senabi kaz 'that girl', KLY sepalab kaazi, KKY sepalbi kaz 'those two children', sethabi kœzil 'those children'); kedha 'like this/that, thus' can also take this suffix (e.g. kedhabi puy 'such a tree').

    Case/Suffixhere
    non specific
    here
    specific
    there
    non-specific
    there
    specific
    nom-acc kaiin masc,
    ina fem,
    ipal dual,
    itha pl
    sei,
    senau masc,
    sena/sina fem,
    sepal/sipal dual,
    setha/sitha pl
    inst kedha kedha
    gen kœu, kœwau seu, sewau
    dat kœpa, kœwupa sepa/sipa, sewupa
    abl kœzi, kœwuzi seizi/sizi, sewuzi
    sp-loc kai, kœwain masc,
    ina fem,
    ipal dual,
    itha pl
    sei,
    sí, sewa
    senau masc,
    sena/sina fem,
    sepal/sipal dual,
    setha/sitha pl
    n-sp-loc kaiki, kawuki/kœwukiinuki masc,
    inaki fem,
    ipalki dual,
    ithaki pl
    seiki/siki, sewukisenauki masc,
    senaki/sinaki fem,
    sepalki/sipalki dual,
    sethaki/sithaki pl
    sim/gl-loc kedhakedhakedhakedha
    article (simulative article)

    kedhabi

    inubi masc,
    inabi fem,
    ipalbi dual,
    ithabi pl
    (simulative article)

    kedhabi

    senaubi masc,
    senabi/sinabi fem,
    sepalbi/sipalbi dual,
    sethabi/sithabi pl
    The other demonstratives
    Case/Suffixguika(rai)ngapapai/papun/pawa
    nom-acc-inst-sp-loc specific[lower-alpha 1] (pi)nugui masc,
    (pi)nagui fem,
    (pi)palgui dual,
    (pi)thagui pl
    (pi)nuka masc,
    (pi)naka fem,
    (pi)palka dual,
    (pi)thaka pl
    (pi)nungap masc,
    (pi)nangap fem,
    (pi)palngap dual,
    (pi)thangap pl
    (pi)nupai masc,
    (pi)napai fem,
    (pi)palpai dual,
    (pi)thapai pl
    (pi)nupun masc,
    (pi)napun fem,
    (pi)palpun dual,
    (pi)thapun pl
    nom-acc-inst-loc non-specific kaiguikaikakaingapkaipai/kaipaipakaipun, kaipawapa
    dat specific[lower-alpha 1] (pi)numulupa masc,
    (pi)namulupa fem,
    (pi)palmulupa dual,
    (pi)thamulupa pl
    (pi)nukaripa masc,
    (pi)nakaripa fem,
    (pi)palkaripa dual,
    (pi)thakaripa pl
    (pi)nungapapa masc,
    (pi)nangapapa fem,
    (pi)palngapapa dual,
    (pi)thangapapa pl
    (pi)nupaipa masc,
    (pi)napaipa fem,
    (pi)palpaipa dual,
    (pi)thapaipa pl
    (pi)nupawapa masc,
    (pi)napawapa fem,
    (pi)palpawapa dual,
    (pi)thapawapa pl
    non-specific dat mulupakaraipa/kadaipakaingapapa(kai)paipa(kai)pawapa
    abl kiziguikizikakizingapkizipaikizipun
    n-sp-loc/gl-loc neutral[lower-alpha 1] (pi)nuguiki masc,

    (pi)naguiki fem,
    (pi)palguiki dual,
    (pi)thaguiki pl

    (pi)nukaki masc,
    (pi)nakaki fem,
    (pi)palkaki dual,
    (pi)thakaki pl
    (pi)nungapaki masc,
    (pi)nangapaki fem,
    (pi)palngapaki dual,
    (pi)thangapaki pl
    (pi)nupaiki/(pi)nupaipa masc,
    (pi)napaiki/(pi)napaipa fem,
    (pi)palpaiki/(pi)palpaipa dual,
    (pi)thapaiki/(pi)thapaipa pl
    (pi)nupuniki/(pi)nupawapa masc,
    (pi)napuniki/(pi)napawapa fem,
    (pi)palpuniki/(pi)palawapa dual,
    (pi)thapuniki/(pi)thapawapa pl
    n-sp/gl-loc kaiguikikaikakikaingapakikaipaiki/kaipaipakaipunki, kaipawapa
    1. Forms without the pi prefix are more pronominal in function.

    Pronouns


    The personal pronouns are three-way nominative-ergative-accusative in declension. Note that the third person pronouns are also used as definite articles, e.g. Nuidh garkœzin nan yipkaz imadhin 'The man saw the woman'.

    Case/SuffixI/meyouhe/it
    (the)
    she/it
    (the)
    whowhat
    nom ngainginuinangami- (miai, miza)
    acc ngœnanginnuinnannganmi- (miai, miza);
    min
    inst ngathngidhnuidhnadhngadhmidh (miaidu/miden/midu/midun, mizœpun)
    gen ngau masc, ngœzu femnginunungunanungœnumingu (miaingu, mizœngu)
    dat ngayapangibepanubepanabepangabepamipa (miaipa, mizœpa)
    abl ngaungu(z) masc, ngœzungu(z) femnginungu(z)nungungu(z)nanungu(z)ngœnungu(z)mingu(zi) (miaingu, mizœngu)
    sp-loc ngaibiyangibiyanubiyanabiyangabiyamiaide/miainu,
    mizœpunu
    n-sp-loc ngaibiyangibiyanubiyanabiyangabiyamiaiya,
    mizœpuya
    gl-loc ngaibiyangibiyanubiyanabiyangabiyamiaiyab,
    mizœpuyab
    proprietive/plural midel, mizœpul
    priv ngaugi masc, ngœzugi femnginuginunguginanugingœnugimiaigi,
    mizœgi
    sim ngaudh masc, ngœzudh femnginudhnungudhnanudhngœnudhmidh (miaidh, mizœpudh)
    res miaizi, mizœzi

    Dual pronouns

    The dual and plural pronouns are nominative-accusative, the accusative being the same in form as the genitive, except in KKY, where the accusative is unmarked.

    Case/Suffixwe dualyou and Iyou dualthem dual
    (the dual)
    who dual
    nom-acc-inst ngalbengœbangipelpalai
    (Boigu pale)
    ngawal
    gen ngalbenngœbanngipenpalamun
    (Boigu palemun)
    (as for singular)
    dat ngalbelpangœbalpangipelpapalamulpa
    (Boigu palemulpa)
    (as for singular)
    abl ngalbelngungœbalngungipelngupalamulngu
    (Boigu palemulngu)
    (as for singular)
    loc ngalbeniyangœbaniyangipeniyapalamuniya
    (Boigu palemuniya)
    (as for singular)
    sim ngalbedhngœbadhngipedhpalamudh
    (Boigu palemudh)
    (as for singular)

    Ngawal 'who dual' is constructed from nga 'who' plus the clitic -wal 'both (dual conjunction)'.


    Plural pronouns

    Case/Suffixwe (exclusive)we (inclusive)youthey
    (the)
    who
    nom-acc-inst ngœingalpangithathanangaya
    gen ngœimunngalpanngithamunthanamun(as for singular)
    dat ngœimulpangalpalpangithamulpathanamulpa(as for singular)
    abl ngœimulngungalpalngungithamulnguthanamulngu(as for singular)
    loc ngœimuniyangalpaniyangithamuniyathanamuniya(as for singular)
    sim ngœimudhngalpadhngithamudhthanamudh(as for singular)

    Ngaya 'who many' is constructed from nga 'who' plus the clitic -ya 'and others (plural conjunction)'.


    Personal names and familiar kinship terms


    Familiar kinship terms are the equivalent of English kin terms such as Dad and Mum, while non-familiar terms are the equivalent of Father and Mother; these latter are treated as common nouns in the language.

    Case/SuffixTom (mas.)Anai (fem.)Dad/Uncle
    (cf. father/uncle)
    Mum/Aunty
    (cf. mother/aunt)
    nom-inst TomAnaiBáb
    (thathi)
    Ama
    (ápu)
    acc-gen TomanAnainaBaban
    (thathiu)
    Amana
    (apuwau)
    dat TomalpaAnailpaBabalpa
    (thathipa)
    Amalpa
    (apuwapa)
    abl TomalnguAnailnguBabalngu
    (thathingu)
    Amalngu
    (apuwangu)
    loc TomaniyaAnainiyaBabaniya
    (thathiya)
    Amaniya
    (apuwaya)
    proprietive/plural babal
    (thathil)
    amal
    (apuwal)
    priv babagi
    (thathigi)
    amagi
    (apuwagi)
    sim TomadhAnaidhbabadh
    (thathidh)
    amadh
    (apuwadh)
    res babazi
    (thathizi)
    amazi
    (apuwazi)

    Kauřařaigau Ya nominal morphology


    The earliest grammatical records of the language are those of the mid-1800s Kauřařaigau Ya dialect. This dialect is identical to the modern dialects, apart from having more archaic forms of some endings and suffixes as well as stem forms.


    Nominal suffixes and endings

    Common Nominals
    Proper Nominals

    No early writer recorded declined feminine forms, apart from the genitive. Ray (1907:20-21) implies (by default) that the OKY paradigm is basically the same as that of OKLY.


    Kauřařaigau Ya Pronouns

    Brierly (B), MacGillivray (M) and Ray (R) recorded the following forms of the singular pronouns of OKY:

    Nominative
    Accusative
    Instrumental-Ergative
    Genitive

    Based on the above forms and the modern dialects, the OKY pronouns are reconstructed as follows:

    pronounNominativeAccusativeErgative-InstrumentalGenitiveDativeAblativeLocative
    1stngayingœnangathungau masc
    ngœzu fem
    ngaikikangaunguzi masc
    ngœzunguzi fem
    ngaikiya
    2ndnginginangidhunginungibepa[ri]nginunguzingibiya
    3rd masculinenuinuinanuidhunungunubepa[ri]nungunguzinubiya
    3rd femininenanananadhunanunabepa[ri]nanunguzinabiya
    whonganganangadhungœnungabepa[ri]ngœnunguzingabiya
    whatmiyaimiyaimidhumingumipa[ri]minguzimizapuya

    The accusatives, the ablatives and imitatives underwent optional final vowel deletion, while the ergatives optionally transformed the final u to a or œ, or deleted it, thus ngathu > ngatha > ngathœ > ngath.

    The recorded dual-plural forms are:

    Nominative-Ergative-Instrumental


    Accusative-Genitive


    Dative


    Ablative

    These can be reconstructed as:

    personNominative-Ergative-InstrumentalAccusative-GenitiveDativeAblativeLocativeImitative-Similative
    1st Dualngalbaingalbainingalbainipangalbainingu
    ngalbainungu
    ngalbainiyangalbainidha
    1st-2nd Dualngabangabaningabanipangabaningu
    ngabanungu
    ngabaniyangabanidha
    2nd Dualngipelngipeningipenipangipeningu
    ngipenungu
    ngipeniyangipenidha
    3rd Dualpalai
    pale
    palamùnipalamùnipapalamùningu
    palamùnungu
    palamùniyapalamùnidha
    1st Pluralngœřingœři(mù)ningœři(mù)nipangœři(mù)ningu
    ngœři(mù)nungu
    ngœři(mù)niyangœři(mù)nidha
    1st-2nd Pluralngalpangalpa(mù)ningalpa(mù)nipangalpa(mù)ningu
    ngalpa(mù)nungu
    ngalpa(mù)niyangalpa(mù)nidha
    2nd Pluralngitha(na)ngitha(na)(mù)ningitha(na)(mù)nipangitha(na)(mù)ningu
    ngitha(na)(mù)nungu
    ngitha(na)(mù)niyangitha(na)(mù)nidha
    3rd Pluralthanathana(mù)nithana(mù)nipathana(mù)ningu
    thana(mù)nungu
    thana(mù)niyathana(mù)nidha

    Verb morphology


    Verbs can have over 100 different aspect, tense, voice, mood and number forms. Verb agreement is with the object (i.e. 'ergative') in transitive clauses, and with the subject in intransitive clauses. Imperatives, on the other hand, agree with both subject and object in transitive clauses.

    There are three aspects ('perfective', 'imperfective', 'habitual'), two telicity forms ('active', which focuses on the verb activity and subsumes many intransitives, many antipassives and some transitives, and 'attainative', which subsumes many transitives, some antipassives and some intransitives), two moods ('non-imperative' and 'imperative' [which resembles a subjunctive in some uses]), 6 tenses ('remote future', 'today/near future', 'present', 'today past', 'recent past', 'remote past' — KLY has developed a 7th tense, a 'last night' tense) and four numbers ('singular', 'dual', 'specific plural', 'animate active plural' — in form the animate active plural is the same as the singular, and is only found on certain verbs).

    In most descriptions of the language the active and attainative forms have been mistermed transitive and intransitive respectively. Transitive, intransitive, passive, antipassive and 'antipassive passive' in the language are syntactic categories, and are formed by the interplay of nominal and verbal morphology, clause/sentence-level characteristics such as word-order, and semantic considerations.

    Verb morphology consists of prefixes (aspect, positioning, etc.), suffixes (telicity, number, and two fossilised multiplicative/causative suffixes) and endings (tense, aspect and mood, and a very limited extent number and telicity). The structural matrix of the verb is as follows. Note that the two fossilised suffixes are mutually exclusive; if a suffix is in the A slot, a suffix cannot appear in the B slot, and vice versa:

    (prefix) + (prefix) + stem (+FOSSILISED SUFFIX A) + (TELICITY) (+FOSSILISED SUFFIX B) + (number) + ending (+ending)

    Examples:

    prefix: pa- 'telic prefix'

    prefix: bal- 'positional — across'

    stem: kabutha- 'place, lay'

    telicity suffix: 'attainative', -i 'active'

    number suffix: -ma 'dual' (absolutive agreement)

    tense-aspect-mood ending: -dhin 'remote past perfective'

    prefix: gar- 'collective'

    stem: wœidha- 'place, put'

    Fossilised suffix: ma 'intensive'

    telicity suffix: i 'active'

    number suffix: ma 'dual'

    tense-aspect-mood ending: dhin 'remote past perfective'


    Sample verb declension


    The verb here is íma- 'see, observe, supervise, examine, try, test'

    Tensed forms
    Case/SuffixPerfective AttainativeImperfective AttainativePerfective ActiveImperfective Active
    remote future singular imaneimaipu (imaiparui)imedheimepu (imeparui)
    dual imamaneimampu (imamparui)imemadheimempu (imemparui)
    plural imamœineimamœipu (imamœiparui)imemœidheimemœipu (imemœiparui)
    near future singular imaipaimaipu (imaiparui)imepaimepu (imeparui)
    dual imampaimampu (imamparui)imempaimempu (imemparui)
    plural imamœipaimamœipu (imamœiparui)imemœipaimemœipu (imemœiparui)
    present singular imanimaipaimizimepa
    dual imamanimampaimemanimempa
    plural imamœinimamœipaimemœinimemœipa
    today past singular imanuimadhaimemaimedha
    dual imamanuimamadhaimemanuimemadha
    plural imamœinuimamœidhaimemœinuimemœidha
    recent past singular imanguimarnguimainguimairngu
    dual imamanguimamarnguimemanguimemarngu
    plural imamœinguimamœirnguimemœinguimemœirngu
    remote past singular imadhinimarimaidhinimai
    dual imamadhinimamarimemadhinimemar
    plural imamœidhinimamœi (imamir)imemœidhinimemœi (imemir)
    Non-tensed forms
    Case/SuffixSingularDualPlural
    Attainative Habitual imaipu (imaiparui)imampu (imamparui)imamœipu (imamœiparui)
    Active Habitual imepu (imeparui)imempu (imemparui)imemœipu (imemœiparui)
    Perfective Attainative Imperative (Singular Subject) imarimamarimamœi (imamir)
    (Non-Singular Subject) imau (imaziu)imamariuimamœi (imamœiziu, imamiu)
    Active Imperative imiimemariuimemœi (imemœiziu, imemiu)
    Imperfective Attainative Imperative imadhaimamadhaimamœidha
    Imperfective Active Imperative imedhaimemadhaimemœidha
    Nominalised forms
    Case/SuffixVerbal NounProprietivePrivativeResultative
    unmarked form imaiimailimaigiimaizi
    independent impersonal form (nom-acc) imaiimailngaimaigingaimaizinga
    stem imai-imailmai-imaigimai-imaizimai-
    independent personal form (nom-acc) imailaigimaigigimaizig
    stem imailga-imaigiga-imaiziga-

    Kauřařaigau Ya verbal morphology


    Prefixes

    These were the same as in the modern dialects.

    Suffixes

    The only suffix differences with the modern dialects were in the form of the plural and verbal noun suffixes. In OKY these were maři and ři respectively. The dual was ngauma on ma- 'take, give, move etc.' and otherwise uma.

    Class 1: wœidha- 'put, place, cook'

    Class 2: ni-, niya- 'sit, stay'

    Verb endings

    ATTAINATIVE INDICATIVEperfectivesingular perfective active
    (where different)
    imperfective
    remote future-kœrui -kœrui
    future-pa[ri] -kœrui
    present-nu-izi
    monosyllabic stem: -iziři
    -pa[ri]
    today past-nulai-ma-adha
    recent past-ngùl -r(a)ngùl
    remote past-dhin(i) -r(a)
    ATTAINATIVE IMPERATIVE-r(a) SgS, -u PlS, -riu Dual-i-adha

    On the whole, the OKY verb seems to have been declined like the Kalau Lagau Ya verb. This includes the loss of the suffix ma in the intransitive imperfective present/perfective today future singular. This loss, however, appears to have been optional in the today past equivalent:[17]

    Vowel/diphthong deletion and reduction in class 1b verbs was optional in OKY where it is now optional or obligatory:

    The irregular verb yœwi- / iya- / yœuna- 'lie/slant/lean over/down' was recorded in the form iipa (eepah), indicating the stem ii- (the remote past form iir is found in modern KY, though not recorded in OKY). Otherwise, only yœuna- was recorded for OKY.


    Miscellaneous paradigms


    Three paradigms that have irregular morphology are:


    Sign language


    The Torres Strait Islanders, neighbouring Papuans and neighbouring Australians have a common sign language,[18] though early records did not make a detailed study of this (e.g. Australian Aboriginal sign languages).[19] Simple conversations and stories can be carried out in the sign language; however, it does not attain the sophistication of a fully developed sign language. It's had some influence on Far North Queensland Indigenous Sign Language.


    See also



    References


    1. ABS. "Census 2016, Language spoken at home by Sex (SA2+)". stat.data.abs.gov.au. Retrieved 30 October 2017.
    2. Y1 Kalau Lagau Ya at the Australian Indigenous Languages Database, Australian Institute of Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Studies
    3. Y1 Kalaw Lagaw Ya at the Australian Indigenous Languages Database, Australian Institute of Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Studies
    4. Mitchell 2015.
    5. Mitchell 1995, p. 9.
    6. Bouckaert, Remco R.; Bowern, Claire; Atkinson, Quentin D. (April 2018). "The origin and expansion of Pama–Nyungan languages across Australia". Nature Ecology & Evolution. 2 (4): 741–749. doi:10.1038/s41559-018-0489-3. PMID 29531347. S2CID 4208351.
    7. Bruno McNiven et al. 2004.
    8. cf. Dutton & 1972 & 1976, Verhoeve 1982
    9. Mitchell 1995.
    10. Alpher et al. 2008. Torres Strait Language Classification. in Bowern, Evans, and Miceli (eds). _Morphology and Language History_ Amsterdam: John Benjamins
    11. Wurm 1975, pp. 333–334
    12. Thomason & Kaufmann 1988, p. 212
    13. Laade 1968.
    14. Ngajedan 1987.
    15. "Masig calendar - Indigenous Weather Knowledge". Bureau of Meteorology. Retrieved 16 July 2020.
    16. Lawrence 1989.
    17. MacGillivray 1852, p. 311.
    18. Seligman, C. G., and A. Wilkin (1907). The gesture language of the Western Islanders, in "Reports of the Cambridge Anthropological Expedition to Torres Straits." Cambridge, England: The University Press, v.3.
    19. Kendon, A. (1988) Sign Languages of Aboriginal Australia: Cultural, Semiotic and Communicative Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press

    Bibliography



    На других языках


    - [en] Kalaw Lagaw Ya

    [es] Kalaw Lagaw Ya

    Kalau Lagau Ya, Kalaw Lagaw Ya, Kala Lagaw Ya (/kala(u) laɡau ja/), o el idioma del Estrecho de Torres occidental (también varios otros nombres, ver abajo), es el idioma indígena del centro y oeste de las Islas del Estrecho de Torres, Queensland, Australia. En algunas islas, ahora ha sido reemplazado en gran parte por el criollo del Estrecho de Torres.

    [ru] Кала-лагав-я

    Кала-лагав-я — австралийский язык пама-ньюнгской языковой семьи. На языке говорят аборигены островов Торресова пролива. Острова принадлежат Австралии и располагаются между ней и Папуа — Новой Гвинеей. Число носителей — 1216 человек[1]. Другая часть аборигенов говорит на языке мериам-мир, который относится не к языкам Австралии, а к папуасским языкам.



    Текст в блоке "Читать" взят с сайта "Википедия" и доступен по лицензии Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike; в отдельных случаях могут действовать дополнительные условия.

    Другой контент может иметь иную лицензию. Перед использованием материалов сайта WikiSort.org внимательно изучите правила лицензирования конкретных элементов наполнения сайта.

    2019-2024
    WikiSort.org - проект по пересортировке и дополнению контента Википедии